Sponsored by the HealthcareTrainingInstitute.org providing Quality Education since 1979
Add to Shopping Cart

Section 17
Materialism & Money Spending Disposition

CEUs Question 17 | CEUs Answer Booklet | Table of Contents | Addictions CEU Courses
Social Worker CEU, Psychologist CE, Counselor CEU, MFT CEU

This research explored the relationships between materialism and money spending attitudes on impulse buying tendencies, attitudes toward debt, sensation seeking, and openness to experience. Students and other adults (N = 266) completed a materialism scale, portions of two money conservation scales, an impulse buying scale, an attitudes toward debt scale, a sensation seeking scale, and an openness to experience scale. Simultaneous-entry multiple regression analyses revealed that materialism and money conservation were predictive of impulse buying, sensation seeking, and openness to experience. Two marginally significant interactions emerged. Individuals less materialistic and tight with money had particularly negative attitudes toward debt, and individuals less materialistic and loose with money were particularly open to experience. Results are discussed with respect to how materialism may be related to a variety of individual difference variables, both at the main effect level and in interaction with money spending attitudes.

Tatzel (2002) proposed a taxonomy of "money worlds" in which she integrated the notions of materialism and money spending disposition. In her taxonomy, she cogently articulated how money spending penchants may combine with materialism to predict a number of other phenomena. The purpose of the current research is to empirically test some of the predictions she made.

Materialism is "… a value representing the individual's orientation toward the role of possessions in life, serving to guide the types and quantities of goods purchased" (Mick, 1996, p. 108). To a great extent, research on materialism has tended to focus on materialism as a main effect. However, at least two studies have examined interactive effects of materialism. Burroughs and Rindfleisch (2002) examined how ownership of material objects and its negative relationship with well-being is a function of other important life values. Also, LaBarbera and Gürhan (1997) found that people high in materialism with lower incomes had lower levels of well-being than people high in materialism with higher incomes. Materialism at the main effect level has been a topic of a comprehensive study of how materialism is related to spending tendencies, saving, and debt. Watson (2003) found that materialistic people tended to be more likely to spend money, more likely to express positive attitudes toward borrowing money for luxury purchases, and less likely to own vehicles of savings (e.g., mutual funds) than were less materialistic people. The paucity of research on the interactions between materialism and other phenomena warrants further research.

To better understand the different meanings that money holds for people, empirical research has focused primarily on the development of scales to assess such differences (e.g., Furnham, 1984; Tang, 1992; Yamauchi & Templer, 1982). These undertakings have each demonstrated that the meanings people ascribe to money are indeed diverse. For instance, in a factor analysis, Furnham (1984) found six distinct factors of money beliefs and behaviors, including obsession, power/spending, retention, security/conservatism, inadequacy, and effort/ability. In the current research, we were particularly interested in people's tendency to conserve (or not conserve) money, and how such a tendency may combine with materialistic values to predict economic attitudes (i.e., attitudes toward debt and impulse buying) and personality traits (i.e., sensation seeking and openness to experience).

Money Attitudes, Materialism, and Tatzel’s (2002) Taxonomy
Tatzel (2002) proposed a litany of outcomes based on individuals' materialistic orientation and money spending penchants. She outlined the characteristics of four possible combinations of these variables as seen in Table 1. With respect to people who are loose with money, Tatzel distinguished between big spenders and experiencers. Big spenders are highly materialistic people who enjoy spending money, often view higher prices as a quality signifier, and who enjoy owning nice possessions. In contrast, experiencers are less materialistic individuals who are willing to spend money on such things as travel or free dining, but do not seek to own expensive or lavish goods. Van Boven and Gilovich (2003) found distinct differences between purchases with the primary intention of acquiring life experience (e.g., a hiking trip) and purchases with the primary intention of acquiring material goods or objects kept in possession (e.g., a flat screen television). That is, some people may seek an outcome that is an experience (and therefore not tangible) and some people may seek an outcome that is a product (and therefore tangible). Other research (Holt, 1995) has predicted that those who are non-materialists have consumption styles conducive to experience and play. With respect to people who are tight with money, Tatzel (2002) distinguished between value seekers and non-spenders. Value seekers are highly materialistic people who dislike spending money, but enjoy owning nice possessions. Such individuals are often collectors, seeking a bargain to "show off." They are likely to save money so that they can buy products of high quality. In contrast, non-spenders are less materialistic, exhibiting anxiety toward spending money, and enjoyment in "watching" their money (Tatzel, p. 119). Such individuals are likely to be ascetic in their lifestyles and perceived by others as "misers" (Tatzel, p. 120). Whereas such individuals may have ample financial resources, they do not crave possessions, and prefer not to spend money.

Impulsive Buying and Attitudes Toward Debt
Impulse buying includes both a lack of planning or deliberation before purchasing a product and an emotional response accompanying the purchases. Instances of impulse buying often stem from buying products for 'non-rational' reasons, such as to relieve a depressed mood, to express identity, or just for fun (Verplanken & Herabadi, 2001). Such behaviors would seem to indicate looseness with one's money and materialistic tendencies (see Table 2 for a diagram of our predictions). Big spenders' low inhibition to spend money and highly materialistic orientation could lead to a buy-now-pay-later approach (Heslin, Johnson, & Blake, 1989), which would likely lead to a positive attitude toward debt.

In contrast, value seekers, with a "save-to-spend" mindset and a penchant to "bargain-hunt" (Tatzel, 2002) may demonstrate an aversion to debt and a tendency to shun impulse purchases. Likewise, nonspenders are also likely to be adverse to debt. They have no reason to borrow money to buy things and instead enjoy accumulating money, rather than spending it on possessions. Additionally, we speculate that non-spenders would seem unlikely to engage in impulse buying because doing so would mean not only parting unexpectedly with their money, but would also result in the accumulating of possessions that are not necessary to their existence.

Openness to Experience and Sensation Seeking
In the current research, we examined how money spending disposition and materialism might predict sensation seeking and openness to experience. Zuckerman (1979) defined sensation seeking as "the need for varied, novel and complex sensations and experiences, and the willingness to take physical and social risks for the sake of such experience" (p. 10). Openness to experience is a person's tendency to be curious, imaginative, and unconventional. Though they may tap into similar constructs, previous research (e.g., Zuckerman, Kuhlman, Joireman, Teta, & Kraft, 1993) has indicated that openness to experience and sensation seeking are not strongly correlated (r = .13).

Research has revealed a relationship between the constructs of sensation seeking and openness to experience with economic variables. For instance, Wong and Carducci (1991) found that high sensation seekers have greater financial risk-taking tendencies in everyday money matters, i.e., personal investments, household affairs, and gambling. Likewise, in an examination of farmers' business-related behaviors and personality traits, Austin, Deary, and Willock (2001) indicated that farmers who were more open to experience also displayed more production-oriented behaviors, i.e., behaviors that are indicative of a profitable business.

To sensation seek, e.g., by going surfing (Diehm & Armatas, 2004), or to enjoy new experiences, e.g., enjoying art and music (Rawlings, Vidal, & Furnham, 2000), would require one to be willing to spend money, but not on material possessions. Indeed, to "experience" a stimulus may mean one has to pay money for it, but cannot physically possess it. In the current investigation, we expect that experiencers, i.e., those loose with money but not materialistic, would be particularly high in sensation seeking and openness to experience.
- Troisi, Jordan, Christopher, Andrew & Pam Marek; Materialism and money spending disposition as predictors of economic and personality variable; North American Journal of Psychology; 2006; Vol. 8; Issue 3.
The article above contains foundational information. Articles below contain optional updates.

Personal Reflection Exercise #10
The preceding section contained information about materialism and money spending disposition. Write three case study examples regarding how you might use the content of this section in your practice.

Online Continuing Education QUESTION 17
According to Meier, what is the difference between big spenders and experiencers? To select and enter your answer go to CEU Answer Booklet.

 

CEU Answer Booklet for this course | Addictions CEU Courses
Forward to Section 18
Back to Section 16
Table of Contents
Top

The article above contains foundational information. Articles below contain optional updates.
Baltimore man behind 27 heroin overdoses, pleads guilty - July 19, 2018
Karon Elijah Peoples pleaded guilty in Baltimore to the distribution of heroin and conspiracy charges this week after drugs he helped distribute were linked to 27 overdoses. Nine of those 27 overdoses were fatal. Police connected the overdoses, deaths, and Peoples by records of messages to and from his large collection of cell phones. According ...
The post Baltimore man behind 27 heroin overdoses, pleads guilty appeared first on Addictions.
CDC Director Robert Redfield says son nearly killed by opioids - July 18, 2018
It’s the job of people at the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention to fight opioid addiction and abuse, but for the agency’s director, the battle is deeply personal. Robert Redfield, the director of the CDC since March, who turned 66 earlier this month, said in a speech at an event last week that ...
The post CDC Director Robert Redfield says son nearly killed by opioids appeared first on Addictions.
Pennsylvania updates opioid prescribing policies - July 17, 2018
Pennsylvania’s governor, Tom Wolf, said July 16 that his state has issued new guidelines about how and when opioid drugs should be prescribed to people with workers’ compensation claims. The state’s health secretary, Rachel Levine, said these new regulations will aid healthcare providers in choosing the right drugs, but they won’t take the place of ...
The post Pennsylvania updates opioid prescribing policies appeared first on Addictions.
7-year-old Detroit girl killed while seeking help after father’s DUI crash - July 16, 2018
A 7-year-old girl was killed by a car in Detroit while trying to cross the highway to get help after her father, who was drunk, crashed his car early Sunday morning. Dessandra Mariah Thomas, police said, was trying to cross the median of Interstate 94 near the Detroit Metro Airport. Her father, driving drunk on ...
The post 7-year-old Detroit girl killed while seeking help after father’s DUI crash appeared first on Addictions.
New York offers medical marijuana as a substitute for opioids - July 13, 2018
The New York State Department of Health will allow people who might otherwise qualify for opioid painkillers to use medical marijuana as replacement therapy and to use pot as a treatment for opioid addiction. This move by the Empire State is an attempt to cut down on the number of opioid painkiller prescriptions and thereby ...
The post New York offers medical marijuana as a substitute for opioids appeared first on Addictions.

CEU Continuing Education for
Social Worker CEU, Psychologist CEU, Counselor CEU, MFT CEU

Get Social Worker CEUs, Psychology CEUs, Marriage and Family Therapist CEUS, Counselor CEUS, or Addiction Counselor CEUs for license renewal. OnlineCECredit.com offers 150+ easy, fast, affordable CE courses to earn your Continuing Education Credit. We are an approved provider of CE courses by APA, ASWB, NBCC, NAADAC, and various State Licensing Boards.

Our objective is to provide you… the Social Worker, Psychologist, Marriage and Family Therapist, Counselor, or Addiction Counselor with continuing education courses that contain practical, how-to interventions. Do these CE courses meet the requirements of your state licensing board? Select your state and profession above to view your Licensing Board's continuing education requirements.

 

OnlineCEUcredit.com Login


Forget your Password Reset it!