There has been much writing, but little research, on the childhood precursors of adult violent behavior. The triad of enuresis,
pyromania, and cruelty to animals (e.g., Heilman and Blackman 1966) is probably
the most frequently cited set of predictors of this sort. One survey (Justice,
Justice and Kraft 1974) reviewed 1,500 references to violence in psychiatric literature,
interviewed over 750 professionals who dealt with violent persons, and retrospectively
analyzed over 1,000 clinical cases to ascertain the most cited childhood predictors
of adult violence. The authors reported that the four early warning signs
were fighting, temper tantrums, school problems, and an inability to get along
with others. The child, in other words, is indeed father or mother to the adult.
Based
on discussions with large groups of psychiatrists and psychologists, Goldstein
(1974) concluded that the agreed upon predictors of violence were
a childhood history of maternal deprivation, poor father identification,
or both; nocturnal enuresis; possibly fire setting; violence toward animals; and
brutalization by one or both parents (p. 27). Diamond (1974) comments that
the conclusion of the clinicians cited by Goldstein represents the sum total of
our present scientific knowledge concerning predictive factors of
murderous violence.
Yet I have repeatedly found some, and sometimes
all of these predictive factors, in individuals who have never committed even
the slightest harmful act, let alone assault or murder. And I have examined
offenders who have committed the extraorçlinarily brutal acts of great
violence and lethality who possessed none of these factors. (Diamond 1974, p.
444.)
One of the most famous studies of the childhood
correlates of later criminal behavior is Unraveling Juvenile Delinquency,
published by Glueck and Glueck in 1950. While not concerned specifically with
violent criminality, the Gluecks claimed that three factorssupervision by
the mother, discipline by the mother, and cohesiveness of the familywere
predictive of later crime in young adolescent boys. This research is among the
most methodologically criticized in all of criminology, and there appears to be
a consensus that the practical utility of the Glueck factors in predicting criminality
is marginal at best.
Lefkowitz, Eron, Walder, and Huesman (1977)
published the results of a longitudinal study entitled Growing Up To Be Violent.
This research followed a sample of over 400 males and females in Columbia County,
New York, from ages 8 to 19. They used peer ratings, parent ratings, self-report,
and a personality test to measure aggressive behavior. Lefkowitz
and his coworkers found that aggression at age 8 is the best predictor we
have of aggression at age 19, irrespective of IQ, social class, or parents
aggressiveness (p. 192). Several other variables, among them the fathers
upward social mobility, low identification of the child with his/her parents,
and a preference on the part of boys for watching violent television programs,
were statistically significant predictors of aggression at age 19. Boys who, in
the third grade, preferred television programs such as Gunsmoke or
Have Gun, Will Travel were rated by their peers 10 years later as
three times as aggressive as boys who, in the third grade, preferred Ozzie
and Harriet, I Love Lucy, or Lawrence Welk. What
is not clear from the study is why an 8-year-old boy would prefer Lawrence
Welk to Have Gun, Will Travel in the first place.
McCord
(1979) has reported on a 30-year follow-up of 201 boys who participated
in the Cambridge-Sommerville Youth Project between 1939 and 1945. She found that
36 percent of the incidence of later violent criminality could be accounted for
by childhood predictive factors. The boys who lacked supervision, whose
mothers lacked self-confidence, who had been exposed to parental conflict and
to aggression were subsequently more convicted for personal crimes
(McCord, 1979, p. 1481).
In what has become the most influential
criminological research of the past decade (Geis and Meier 1978), Wolfgang et
al. (1972) obtained information on all boys born in Philadelphia in 1945 who lived
there between their 10th and 18th birthdays. Of the 9,945 boys studied, 3,475,
or 35 percent, had at least one recorded contact with the police by age 18. Wolfgang
et al. found that the variables of race and socioeconomic status (SES) were most
strongly associated with reported delinquency: 29 percent of the whites, but 50
percent of the nonwhites, and 26 percent of the. higher SES, but 45 percent of
the lower SES boys had an offense record.
Chronic
offenders were defined as those who committed five or more violations. Six hundred
and twenty-seven boys, 6 percent of the sample and 18 percent of the total number
of offenders, were responsible for over one-half of all offenses committed.
Chronic
offenders in the cohort had a greater number of residential moves, lower IQ scores,
a greater percentage classified as retarded, and fewer grades completed than did
either the nonchronic or the one-time offenders, even when race and SES were held
constant (p. 248).
Wolfgang (1977) has updated his research
to include data on the subjects up to age 30. Only 5 percent of the subjects had
an arrest record only as an adult (i.e., after age 18 but not before).
While
most juvenile offenders (61 percent) avoid arrest upon reaching adulthood,
the chances of being an adult offender are almost four times greater if one had
a juvenile record than if one did not. While 6 percent of the sample were chronic
offenders by age 18, 15 percent were chronic by age 30. The probability of future
arrest varied directly with the probability of past arrest: The probability of
a fifth arrest (for any crime not necessarily a violent one) given four priors
was .80; the probability of an eleventh arrest given ten previous arrests was
.90. The probability of a fifth serious (or index) offense with four
prior arrests was .36; the probability of an eleventh serious offense given ten
previous arrests was .42.
During the juvenile years, the subjects
reported committing 8 to 11 serious or index offenses for every time they were
arrested. Adults admitted to between three and six offenses for each recorded
act. The main conclusion one could draw from the research on childhood predictors
of adult violence is that the distinction between childhood and adulthood
is not a particularly meaningful one in terms of violence prediction. The same
factors (e.g., a history of past violence) appear to influence the occurrence
of future violence regardless of age. Age is relevant to the extent that the earlier
one begins a career of violence, the longer and more extensive that history may
be, and as one enters the 30s, maturation processes become salient. The search
for factors that imprint a violent disposition at an early age so
far has produced results that are theoretically interesting but without much practical
significance for prediction in the individual case.
- Monohan, John, Predicting
Violent Behavior, Sage Publications, Inc.: Beverly Hills, 1981.
=================================
Personal
Reflection Exercise #4
The
preceding section contained information about childhood prediction of adult violence.
Write three case study examples regarding how you might use the content of this
section in your practice.
Peer-Reviewed Journal Article References:
Dargis, M., & Koenigs, M. (2017). Witnessing domestic violence during childhood is associated with psychopathic traits in adult male criminal offenders. Law and Human Behavior, 41(2), 173–179.
Harman, J. J., Kruk, E., & Hines, D. A. (2018). Parental alienating behaviors: An unacknowledged form of family violence. Psychological Bulletin, 144(12), 1275–1299.
Kita, S., Hayashi, M., Umeshita, K., Tobe, H., Uehara, N., Matsunaga, M., & Kamibeppu, K. (2020). Intimate partner violence and maternal child abuse: The mediating effects of mothers’ postnatal depression, mother-to-infant bonding failure, and hostile attributions to children’s behaviors. Psychology of Violence, 10(3), 279–289.
Kobayashi, J. E., Bernard, N. K., Nuttall, A. K., Levendosky, A. A., Bogat, G. A., & Lonstein, J. S. (2021). Intimate partner violence and positive parenting across early childhood: Comparing self-reported and observed parenting behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 35(6), 745–755.
Liebschutz, J. M., Buchanan-Howland, K., Chen, C. A., Frank, D. A., Richardson, M. A., Heeren, T. C., Cabral, H. J., & Rose-Jacobs, R. (2018). Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) correlations with prospective violence assessment in a longitudinal cohort. Psychological Assessment, 30(6), 841–845.
Narayan, A. J., Labella, M. H., Englund, M. M., Carlson, E. A., & Egeland, B. (2017). The legacy of early childhood violence exposure to adulthood intimate partner violence: Variable- and person-oriented evidence. Journal of Family Psychology, 31(7), 833–843.
Online Continuing Education QUESTION
11
What are four childhood predictors of adult violence? Record the letter
of the correct answer the .
|